Squatchdetective.com
  • Home
    • The Five Tenets
    • About
  • 🆕Squatch-D's Blog
    • Old Blog Site
  • | Squatch-D TV
    • Squatch-D TV Channel
    • 🆕Squatch-D TV 24-7
  • | Events
  • | Investigations
    • Vermont Chapter
  • | 🆕Resources
    • Squatch-D University
    • Audio Library
  • |🆕Interactive
    • 🆕News Room
    • 🆕Reddit r/Bigfoot Feed
  • | Contact
    • Report Encounter
    • Media Contact Page
    • Booking Steve Kulls
    • General Comments / Questions

​J. W. Burns – The Journalist Who Popularized “Sasquatch”

2/25/2025

0 Comments

 
PictureJ.W. Burns
     John W. Burns (1888–1962), was a Canadian schoolteacher and Indian Agent who worked with First Nations communities in British Columbia.

​     In the 1920s, while teaching at the Chehalis Indian Reserve east of Vancouver, Burns became fascinated by local Indigenous legends of “wild hairy giants” said to roam the forests.

    He earned the trust of Chehalis elders and collected their accounts of these creatures. Rather than keep these stories within the community, Burns wrote them up for a wider audience.
​

    His most famous contribution was bringing the Sasquatch legend to mainstream attention through articles and stories published in Canadian media. This work effectively introduced an obscure Indigenous oral tradition to non-Indigenous readers, making Burns a seminal figure in Sasquatch lore.

Picture
Coining the Term “Sasquatch”

      Burns is widely credited as the first to popularize the term “Sasquatch.” In fact, he effectively coined the word by anglicizing an Indigenous term. The word Sasquatch derives from “sasq’ets,” a word in the local Halkomelem (Coast Salish) language referring to a wild man or hairy giant. Burns heard various Indigenous names for the creature and decided to use a single term for his articles.

       In 1929, he introduced this new term to the public in Maclean’s magazine, in an article titled “Introducing B.C.’s Hairy Giants,” thereby putting “Sasquatch” into print. By compiling First Nations stories and giving the creature a memorable name, Burns ensured that the legend would stick in popular imagination.

        His 1929 article was the first modern account of the Sasquatch and is considered the origin point for all later “Bigfoot” lore in pop culture. Every subsequent mention of Sasquatch or Bigfoot can be traced back to Burns’s storytelling in that era.

Cultural Appropriation and Modern Critiques

     While Burns helped preserve and popularize Indigenous stories, modern perspectives often criticize his work as an example of cultural appropriation. Burns was a non-Native “Indian Agent” retelling sacred First Nations legends at a time when Indigenous peoples were oppressed and even legally barred from practicing elements of their culture.

       Critics note that Burns took Coast Salish stories and put his own name on them, repackaging communal oral traditions as his own discovery. According to one analysis, “J.W. Burns took the Sasquatch story from the Coast Salish people, put his name on it, and submitted it to Maclean’s.”

      Indigenous writers argue that Burns’s retelling distorted and commodified their traditions, presenting them as entertaining curiosities for non-Native readers. Over time, Burns became “obsessed” with the Sasquatch, embellishing the legends with each retelling. As a result, the authentic legend eventually became unrecognizable. This has been cited as a classic case of cultural misappropriation—an example of how Indigenous knowledge was taken and transformed by outsiders.

      At the same time, there is a nuanced view that the Sasquatch story has since been reclaimed by Indigenous communities. The Sts’ailes First Nation, for example, openly celebrate Sasquatch Days and share the Sasq’ets legend on their own terms, something that might not have been possible in the 1930s without Burns’s initial exposure of the legend. Nonetheless, the prevailing modern critique is that Burns’s role in Sasquatch lore came at the expense of Indigenous ownership of their own stories.
PictureCondensed captures of Burn's article.
The 1929 Maclean’s Article and April Fool’s Day

      Burns’s landmark article, “Introducing B.C.’s Hairy Giants,” appeared in the April 1, 1929, issue of Maclean’s magazine. Because it was published on April Fool’s Day, some readers at the time wondered if the wild tale was meant as a prank.

     In fact, the piece was a sincere collection of accounts, not a satirical hoax but the coincidence of the date caused confusion. Historically, Maclean’s did not have a tradition of April Fool’s hoaxes. At the time, it was a serious monthly periodical that was typically released on the first of each month as a matter of routine scheduling.
​

      The April 1929 issue happened to fall on the first, so Burns’s article ran that day by coincidence. Cryptozoology historians have noted that Burns did not intend to create a hoax or a joke creature; he was reporting legends he believed to be real.

Contemporary Reception and Legacy

        At the time of publication, Burns’s Sasquatch writings were received with a mix of intrigue and doubt. The sensational nature of the claims combined with the April 1 issue date led many in the public to view Sasquatch as a curious folk tale rather than a credible reality.

        Beyond local circles in British Columbia, the “hairy giants” were largely thought of as myth. Despite skepticism, the article did spark some interest, and in subsequent decades researchers occasionally cited Burns’s accounts as important early evidence for Sasquatch reports. Burns himself continued writing on the subject and helped organize Sasquatch-themed events in Harrison Hot Springs.

     In the years since, J.W. Burns has become known as the “father of Sasquatch.” Cryptozoologists revere his work as foundational, while mainstream scientists and the broader public often view Sasquatch as purely mythical.
Modern scholars also focus on the cultural cost of his writings, noting how he extracted Indigenous legends for a sensational magazine piece and thereby appropriated stories.
​
      Nonetheless, virtually all modern Sasquatch or Bigfoot lore traces back to Burns’s 1929 article and the term he introduced. His role in shaping the legend, despite controversies, remains profound.

      Below you will find both the original issue of MacLean's Magazine and a transcription of Burn's groundbreaking yet controversial article. 

Transcribed JW Burns' Article

Your browser does not support viewing this document. Click here to download the document.

MacLean's Magazine, April 1st, 1929

Your browser does not support viewing this document. Click here to download the document.

​References
  1. Burns, J. W. “Introducing B.C.’s Hairy Giants,” Maclean’s, April 1, 1929.
  2. The Canadian Encyclopedia – Sasquatch.
  3. Kook Science Archive – J. W. Burns.
  4. Outpost Magazine – “Of Myths and Legends in the Great Bear Rainforest” .
  5. Mysteries of Canada – “How the Sasquatch Got Its Name” .
  6. The Walrus – “On Cultural Appropriation, Canadians Are Hypocrites” 
  7. Canadaland – “How Sasquatch was Stolen.”
  8. Cryptomundo – “All-Time Best Cryptozoology April Fools’ Jokes.” 
0 Comments

Unraveling the Mystery of Bigfoot’s Language: A Summary of New Insights

2/24/2025

0 Comments

 

            As a longtime investigator of the Bigfoot phenomenon, I've dedicated much of my career to exploring every angle surrounding this mysterious creature, especially the possibility that Bigfoot could possess speech comparable to that of humans. Recently, I published a paper titled, “Assessing the Unlikelihood of Bigfoot Utilizing Speech Similar to Homo sapiens,” which dives deeply into the biological, evolutionary, and behavioral evidence surrounding this question.

          I’d like to offer a more accessible overview of the paper’s key points and why I believe it’s unlikely that Bigfoot would speak in a manner like ours.

 
Why Speech Matters


         Speech is one of the most defining characteristics of modern humans (Homo sapiens). Our ability to produce a wide range of vocalizations is tied to specific anatomical features, including a descended larynx and specialized brain structures (such as Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas).

          These adaptations co-evolved with other critical developments like advanced tool use, control of fire, and complex social structures. When it comes to Bigfoot, there is no clear evidence of these same developments, so from a purely evolutionary standpoint, the building blocks for Homo sapiens-level speech might simply not be there.

 
Key Findings from the Paper
 
  • Anatomical and Cognitive Requirements: Humans can produce spoken language because of unique anatomical and neurological adaptations. While Bigfoot—if real—may share some traits with other large primates or early hominins, there’s no solid evidence it has the same vocal tract configurations or the advanced brain regions necessary for our level of speech.
 
  • Tool Use and Fire: Throughout human evolution, our use of tools and mastery of fire went hand-in-hand with developing more complex social and communication skills. There’s a strong correlation between these milestones and the selective pressures that shaped language. Reported Bigfoot activity does not consistently show evidence of complex toolmaking or fire usage, making the evolutionary leap to advanced language less likely.
 
  • Behavioral Observations: Sightings and anecdotal reports, while fascinating, typically depict Bigfoot as elusive, employing howls, whoops, or other calls—not structured language. Most of these stories lack the rigorous evidence we’d need to conclude Bigfoot has a communication system on par with human speech.
 
Scott Nelson’s Analysis and the Sierra Sounds

       The most prominent audio recordings that proponents claim could indicate Bigfoot language are the “Sierra Sounds,” captured in the 1970s. Scott Nelson, a retired military cryptolinguist, analyzed these vocalizations and proposed they represent a unique language.

       However, his conclusions rest on a single set of audio samples, which have never been independently validated under scientific scrutiny. Although intriguing, the data remains inconclusive at best.

 
The Role of Skepticism and Scientific Rigor 

     Being skeptical doesn’t mean rejecting the possibility of Bigfoot Speech outright; rather, it ensures that claims about Bigfoot’s biology or behaviors stand up to scientific standards. I emphasize that we need multiple lines of evidence peer-reviewed and independently replicated to confidently claim something as significant as human-like speech in an undiscovered primate.
​

     For those of us who steadfast believe of Bigfoot’s existence, maintaining a rigorous scientific approach to investigating vocalizations is the most productive path forward. With careful methodology and unbiased analysis, we can continue to search for answers while staying grounded in the best evidence available.
​

Your browser does not support viewing this document. Click here to download the document.
0 Comments

Reframing the Bear Misidentification Debate

2/22/2025

0 Comments

 
PictureAmerican Black Bear (Ursus americanus) Photo Credit - Mykola Swarnyk via Wikimedia Commons.
            For those of us who believe in the existence of Bigfoot, skepticism is nothing new. Various explanations have been floated—ranging from hoaxes to misidentified wildlife—but what about the growing body of evidence suggesting that Bigfoot really is out there?

             In A Critical Examination of the Bear Misidentification Claim made by Floe Foxon, I delve into the argument set forth by Floe Foxon’s 2024 paper, Bigfoot: If It’s There, Could It Be a Bear?, which proposes that most sightings are simply black bear misidentifications.

           While there’s no question bears can be confused for other creatures under poor conditions, and may account for some misidentifications, my analysis highlights where Foxon’s approach falls short and why we need to take seriously the evidence that Bigfoot may indeed be a living, as-yet undocumented hominid.


How the Bear Theory Falls Short
  1. Data Gaps:
    Foxon leans heavily on a single year of bear population estimates (2006). Such a narrow window ignores ever-shifting black bear demographics, habitat changes, and conservation efforts. Relying on outdated or incomplete data can create an inflated impression that bears perfectly align with Bigfoot sighting hotbeds.
  2. Regional Mismatches:
    Some states with significant Bigfoot sightings—like Texas, Ohio, and Florida—don’t have extensive black bear populations. Florida’s “Skunk Ape” has been reported in marshy Everglade regions where bears are relatively rare. Ohio has a long history of robust Sasquatch encounters despite only modest bear presence.
  3. Confirmation Bias:
    By focusing on bears as the primary culprit, Foxon effectively dismisses other explanations or evidence that simply doesn’t match the black bear profile. This narrow view can lead researchers to disregard legitimate sightings in “bear-poor” states or to downplay physical evidence—such as footprints with dermal ridges or eyewitness descriptions that don’t match typical bear behavior.

Picture
Picture
The Evidence for Bigfoot’s Existence

Open-minded researchers point to a range of data supporting the notion of a genuine, undiscovered hominid species in North America:
  1. Footprints with Unique Morphology
    Footprint casts often show details like toes with evident flexion and dermal ridges that differ significantly from any known bear paw shape. These footprints frequently appear in remote, hard-to-access regions, making intentional hoaxes less likely.
  2. Hair Samples and Possible DNA
    Over the years, hair samples have been tested by various labs. While some are inconclusive or match known animals, a few defy easy classification. Even though the science is ongoing and no definitive “Bigfoot DNA” has yet been isolated, these anomalies warrant further study rather than immediate dismissal.
  3. Historical and Cultural Accounts (Anecdotal Evidence)
    Indigenous tribes throughout North America have long-standing traditions of large, hairy hominids inhabiting forested regions. The continuity of these stories—predating modern pop culture—supports the idea that Bigfoot could indeed be more than myth.
  4. Witness Evidence and Recordings 
    Many witnesses describe upright, ape-like figures that move in ways atypical for a bear. Alleged audio recordings  capture vocalizations that some experts have difficulty attributing to known wildlife. While witness evidence isn’t necessarily conclusive on its own, the sheer volume of consistent eyewitness reports, with consistent primate behavioral descriptions, across different regions and eras adds weight.
  5. Ecological Plausibility
    Some primatologists and anthropologists argue that a relic hominid could exist in the vast, densely forested areas of the Pacific Northwest and other remote regions. The fact that it hasn’t been fully documented does not, in itself, disprove its existence—large, elusive species have evaded definitive classification before (consider the Okapi, once deemed mythical until it was scientifically recognized).

Why Bear-Based Explanations Aren’t Enough
  • Overlooking Inconvenient Sightings: Foxon’s bear hypothesis fails to accommodate large numbers of sightings in areas with low or non-existent black bear populations.
  • Ignoring Other Large Mammals: There are other animals—like elk, moose, or even feral hogs—that might also complicate wildlife identifications, but none so easily explains the consistent humanoid shape and behavior that Bigfoot witnesses describe.
  • Dismissing Compelling Clues: Physical evidence—from footprints to alleged DNA—cannot be explained by bears alone. Strict reliance on the bear theory can lead researchers to ignore or ridicule data that points to an unknown primate.

A More Holistic Research Path

Instead of attributing every sighting to bears, researchers should:
  • Incorporate Multi-Year Data: Examine trends in bear populations over time, not just a single static snapshot.
  • Account for Regional Variations: Properly investigate locations where bears are uncommon; examine if sightings are consistent with bear-like features or if they suggest a unique phenomenon.
  • Engage in Rigorous Fieldwork: Collect and analyze footprints, audio, video, hair, and other trace evidence to see if it deviates from known species.
  • Balance Skepticism with Open Inquiry: Avoid dismissing unusual findings prematurely. Skepticism is healthy, but it should not shut the door on genuinely unexplained phenomena.

Researchers have long argued that Bigfoot represents more than wishful thinking or misidentified wildlife. A Critical Examination of the Bear Misidentification Claim made by Floe Foxon underscores the importance of thorough, open-minded research. While black bears undoubtedly contribute to some mistaken sightings, the consistent sightings in low-bear regions, coupled with compelling physical and cultural evidence, indicate there’s more to the story than the “it’s all bears” narrative.

 In my latest paper, A Critical Examination of the Bear Misidentification Claim made by Floe Foxon, I take a closer look at the popular assumption that black bear sightings account for the majority of Bigfoot reports. Floe Foxon’s 2024 study attempted to present a compelling argument linking Sasquatch encounters with black bear populations, yet my research uncovers notable inconsistencies and outliers—especially in regions with few bears.

By spotlighting these discrepancies and calling for a broader, data-driven approach, I hope to add depth and nuance to the ongoing discussion about Bigfoot’s potential existence. 


As researchers, if we maintain a robust and unbiased scientific approach, we stand a chance at uncovering the truth behind one of the greatest mysteries of all time! ​
Your browser does not support viewing this document. Click here to download the document.
0 Comments

Bigfoot by the Numbers

2/21/2025

0 Comments

 


              While witness sightings flood online forums and historical records, a deeper numerical analysis provides a clearer picture of Bigfoot’s presence across the United States. Leveraging comprehensive datasets from multiple sources, including the Bigfoot Field Researchers Organization (BFRO), The Bigfoot Mapping Project, Oregon Bigfoot, The Squatchdetective Archives and Gulf Coast Bigfoot Research Organization (GCBRO), we analyze where, when, and under what conditions these sightings occur.

              This data-driven approach highlights the frequency of reports, the states where Bigfoot is most seen, and other factors such as weather, time of day, and even the reported smell. Let's dive into Bigfoot by the Numbers and explore the patterns behind one of the world's greatest mysteries.

State-by-State Bigfoot Sightings
             Analyzing 5,577 reported Bigfoot sightings, we see a clear geographical pattern emerging. Certain states, particularly those with dense forests and vast wilderness, report significantly more sightings than others. Here’s a breakdown of the top states for Bigfoot sightings:
  1. Washington – 724 sightings (12.76%)
  2. California – 463 sightings (8.16%)
  3. Florida – 344 sightings (6.06%)
  4. Ohio – 326 sightings (5.75%)
  5. Illinois – 304 sightings (5.36%)
  6. Oregon – 260 sightings (4.58%)
  7. Texas – 259 sightings (4.57%)
  8. Michigan – 225 sightings (3.97%)
At the other end of the spectrum, states with fewer forests and smaller rural areas report the least number of sightings, such as Delaware (5 sightings) and Rhode Island (5 sightings), which account for just 0.09% of the total reports.


When Are Bigfoot Sightings Most Common?
                Despite the wealth of sighting reports, aggregated monthly data remains elusive. However, researchers like Charles Holbert suggest that late summer and early fall are peak times for sightings. This aligns with the idea that Bigfoot is more likely to be seen when people are hiking, camping, and spending more time in wooded areas.
​
Time of Day Analysis
                The best time to see Bigfoot? Dawn and dusk. Anecdotal reports consistently indicate that Bigfoot sightings occur during low-light conditions, likely due to their nocturnal or crepuscular nature. The Sasquatch Data Project found that nighttime sightings are significantly more common than daytime ones, further supporting the idea that Bigfoot is most active during twilight hours.

What Does Bigfoot Look (and Smell) Like?
While the stereotypical image of Bigfoot is a large, dark-furred creature, the data suggests variability in its appearance:
  • Predominant fur colors: Black, brown, and reddish-brown
  • Less common colors: Grey, white, or a mix
  • Estimated height: 6 to 9 feet tall
               Perhaps the most fascinating aspect of these reports is the common mention of a foul smell. Descriptions range from “rotting meat” to “sulfur”, and 12.5% of Class A sightings (where the creature was seen clearly) included a mention of a strong, putrid odor.
              This matches reports from BFRO case studies and historical encounters, where the smell is often described as overpowering. Whether this is a defense mechanism, a trait of the species, remains unclear.

Bigfoot Sightings by State

Picture

Bigfoot Sightings by Month

Picture

Bigfoot Sightings by Time Period

Picture

Bigfoot Sightings by Weather 

Picture

Bigfoot Sightings by Color Reported

Picture
Bigfoot Sightings – A Data-Driven Conclusion
This numerical deep dive into Bigfoot sightings underscores several key takeaways:

✅ Bigfoot sightings are geographically concentrated in forested states like Washington, California, and Ohio, with Washington leading by a significant margin.
✅ Bigfoot is most often seen in late summer to early fall, aligning with increased outdoor activity.
✅ Twilight and nighttime are the most common times for encounters, possibly due to Bigfoot’s nocturnal nature.
✅ Smell is a frequently reported characteristic, with witnesses describing a sulfuric, musky, or decaying odor in about 1 in 8 sightings.

While skeptics will continue to challenge the existence of Bigfoot, the numbers reveal consistent patterns that suggest something unusual is happening in North America's wilderness. The legend of Bigfoot endures—backed by data and numbers that continue to grow.
Sources & Citations
  • BFRO Geographical Database of Bigfoot Sightings & Reports
  • GCBRO US Database – Gulf Coast Bigfoot Research Organization
  • Bigfoot Encounters State by State Sightings List
  • Squatchdetective Archives
  • 2-D Density Map of Bigfoot Sightings – Charles Holbert
  • Unraveling Bigfoot Hair Color Patterns – Sasquatch Data Project
  • Bigfoot characteristics – Wikipedia
  • What Does Bigfoot Smell Like? – MEL Magazine
0 Comments

    Archives

    June 2025
    May 2025
    April 2025
    March 2025
    February 2025

    RSS Feed

Picture


​Copyright 2006 - 2025

 Steve Kulls / Squatchdetective.com
Readers are free to use materials from this site provided that: 

the information is credited and the original intent, meaning or purpose is not altered.

This website is protected under :
The United States Copyright Act of Fair Use..

Section 107 U.S. Copyright Law: Limitations on exclusive rights:
​ Fair Use 
Notwithstanding the provisions of section 106, the fair use of a copyrighted work, including such use by reproduction in copies or phone records or by any other means specified by that section, for purposes such as criticism, comment, news reporting, teaching (including multiple copies for classroom use), scholarship, or research, is not an infringement of copyright.